Subscribe free to our newsletters via your
. Earth Science News .




BEE STING
Why 'Whispers' Among Bees Sometimes Evolve Into 'Shouts'
by Staff Writers
La Jolla CA (SPX) Jul 08, 2014


Trigona hyalinata spies (shown here) will compete for food sources if their competitors aren't present in sufficient numbers to defend their flowers. Image courtesy Elinor Lichtenberg.

Let's say you're a bee and you've spotted a new and particularly lucrative source of nectar and pollen. What's the best way to communicate the location of this prize cache of food to the rest of your nestmates without revealing it to competitors, or "eavesdropping" spies, outside of the colony?

Many animals are thought to deter eavesdroppers by making their signals revealing the location or quality of resources less conspicuous to outsiders. In essence, they've evolved "whispers" in their signals to counter eavesdropping.

But some species of bees in Brazil do the exact opposite. "Shouts" in their food-recruitment signals warn would-be competitors that their prime source of food will be fiercely defended if they show up to the site. It's a communication strategy that's bold and risky, yet remarkably successful in warding off competitors, according to a paper published in the journal Current Biology.

"It's a signal with honest aspects and the possibility of lies," explains James Nieh, a professor of biology at UC San Diego who oversaw the research study conducted in Brazil by Elinor Lichtenberg, a PhD student in his laboratory who is now a postdoctoral researcher at Washington State University. "It tells nestmates where to find good food and hints at a larger occupying force."

Lichtenberg says her discovery of this counterintuitive method of communication by bees suggests that eavesdroppers can alter the evolution of animal signals in ways that were previously not thought possible.

"Our study provides a new way of looking at how eavesdroppers affect the evolution of animal communication signals," she adds. "Until now, it was thought that eavesdroppers select against conspicuous signals, for example by more easily finding and eating prey that sings loudly. But our results show that eavesdroppers can help select for the same conspicuous signals that are easiest for intended recipients to detect and understand."

To Nieh, whose research has focused on the evolution of communication strategies among bees, "eavesdropping is part of the information web, the signals and cues that surround animals and play a key role in shaping ecosystems."

In the case of bees and other pollinators, he says, "a network of signals and cues shapes pollination, informing animals about where and when food is available. Researchers have in general thought about eavesdropping as a force that makes signals less conspicuous, leading to the evolution of 'whispers' to counter spying. However, we show that eavesdropping can also lead to 'shouts.'

In this stingless bee system, with aggressive colonies jockeying for limited resources, more conspicuous food-recruitment signals indicate a higher likelihood that a resource will be harder to wrest away."

Lichtenberg's study focused on stingless bees-including two from the genus Trigona that recruit nestmates to food sources with chemically distinct pheromones-that compete with one another for similar food sources.

Trigona hyalinata spies that detect food sources marked by Trigona spinipes foragers will often displace T. spinipes from desirable sites in the wild if they can recruit sufficient nestmates. But Lichtenberg found in a controlled field study that the eavesdropping species will avoid desirable sources of food that have been visited frequently by T. spinipes (communicated by the larger number of pheromone markings at the site) to avoid being attacked. However, T. hyalinata foragers are attracted to food sources with fewer T. spinipes foragers.

The eavesdroppers could take over the highly visited sites by recruiting more of their nestmates or battling with T. spinipes bees, which show high levels of aggression toward intruders, but the risks and energy costs to the eavesdroppers apparently aren't worth the trouble.

The researchers supported this hypothesis by modeling eavesdropping bees' decision-making, using a type of model from economics. They ran the model for T. hyalinata eavesdropping on T. spinipes, T. spinipes on T. hyalinata, and the non-aggressive Melipona rufiventris on T. spinipes. In all three cases, they found that the model results matched eavesdropping behavior measured in this study and in previous work by Lichtenberg, Nieh and colleagues.

"Assembling such a group in the nest after having found a food source through eavesdropping uses time and energy the eavesdropper could otherwise spend looking for an unoccupied food source," explains Lichtenberg.

"If the eavesdropper brings too small a group to an occupied food source and cannot win access to it, she and the bees accompanying her have essentially wasted energy. For attacks between colonies of the same species, there is also a risk that the conflict will escalate to physical interactions in which large numbers of bees may die."

"Our study is one of the first to investigate what drives the behavior of eavesdroppers collecting information from competitors within the same trophic level, which use the same food resources as the eavesdropper," she adds.

"Previous eavesdropping research has mainly focused on individuals seeking mates, predators looking for prey or prey trying to avoid being eaten. In those cases, eavesdroppers' expected behavior is clear. This is not true for eavesdropping on competitors."

The study not only provides information about the evolution of different strategies of animal communication, but suggests how these strategies can affect the ecology of plant communities. "Such strategies affect not only the individuals directly involved, but also broader ecological interactions between the food-gatherers and their food," Lichtenberg says.

"This is particularly important for animals such as the bees I studied, because their movements determine plant pollination."

.


Related Links
UC San Diego
The Flowers and Fruits of Earth






Comment on this article via your Facebook, Yahoo, AOL, Hotmail login.

Share this article via these popular social media networks
del.icio.usdel.icio.us DiggDigg RedditReddit GoogleGoogle








BEE STING
Scientists discover bees can reverse brain aging
Phoenix AZ (SPX) Jul 06, 2012
Scientists at Arizona State University have discovered that older honey bees effectively reverse brain aging when they take on nest responsibilities typically handled by much younger bees. While current research on human age-related dementia focuses on potential new drug treatments, researchers say these findings suggest that social interventions may be used to slow or treat age-related dementia ... read more


BEE STING
China gave $14.4 bln in foreign aid in three years

AW139 helicopters to perform emergency medical missions

Accidents raise safety questions on Hong Kong waters

Malaysia to deploy more equipment in MH370 search

BEE STING
Even geckos can lose their grip

Platonic solids generate their four-dimensional analogues

Consider the 'Anticrystal'

Inspired by Nature, Researchers Create Tougher Metal Materials

BEE STING
Rethinking the Reef

Lessons from the West: Great Barrier Reef in danger

New study of largely unstudied mesophotic coral reef geology

Water bonus flows from climate change measures

BEE STING
Japan shipping giant plans first regular Arctic route

Shark teeth offer new look at Arctic climate change

Changing Antarctic winds create new sea level threat

Ancient ocean currents may have changed pace and intensity of ice ages

BEE STING
'Bee-harming' pesticides also hit bird populations: study

Internet crowd bites big into potato salad project

The long, slow march of 'biofortified' GM food

Why does Europe hate GM food and is it about to change its mind?

BEE STING
Rewriting the history of volcanic forcing during the past 2,000 years

Newborn baby among dead as quake hits Mexico, Guatemala

Flooding, havoc in Japan as Typhoon Neoguri batters mainland

Japan issues highest alert over super typhoon Neoguri

BEE STING
Somali capital one step short of famine: UN

Clash between army, 'tribal gunmen' leaves 65 dead in Uganda

Clashes between Nigeria army, Islamists kill 59: official

US military admits secret presence in Somalia

BEE STING
Neandertal trait raises new questions about human evolution

Virtual crowds produce real behavior insights

Insect diet helped early humans build bigger brains

Researchers say hormonal mechanism responsible for left-handedness




The content herein, unless otherwise known to be public domain, are Copyright 1995-2014 - Space Media Network. All websites are published in Australia and are solely subject to Australian law and governed by Fair Use principals for news reporting and research purposes. AFP, UPI and IANS news wire stories are copyright Agence France-Presse, United Press International and Indo-Asia News Service. ESA news reports are copyright European Space Agency. All NASA sourced material is public domain. Additional copyrights may apply in whole or part to other bona fide parties. Advertising does not imply endorsement, agreement or approval of any opinions, statements or information provided by Space Media Network on any Web page published or hosted by Space Media Network. Privacy Statement All images and articles appearing on Space Media Network have been edited or digitally altered in some way. Any requests to remove copyright material will be acted upon in a timely and appropriate manner. Any attempt to extort money from Space Media Network will be ignored and reported to Australian Law Enforcement Agencies as a potential case of financial fraud involving the use of a telephonic carriage device or postal service.